
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
“Statistics are like a bikini. What they reveal is suggestive, but what they conceal is vital,” Aaron Levenstein, a business professor at Baruch College, once said. I first heard a version
of this quote in an undergraduate social psychology class in 2003. Nearly a decade and a half later, psychology is having a replication crisis — and the “bikini” is largely to blame.
Recently, more than 270 psychologists set out to repeat 100 experiments to see if they could generate the same results. They successfully replicated only 39 of the 100 studies. Over several
years, failed attempts to replicate published studies have caused generally accepted bodies of research to be called into question — or rejected outright. One example is the idea that your
willpower is a limited resource that, like a muscle, becomes exhausted when it is used. Another is that power posing — standing like a superhero for two minutes — makes you feel bolder,
reduces stress hormones and increases testosterone. Both have fallen aside due to failed replications. These aren’t dusty, arcane findings limited to academic journals; a TED talk by social
psychologist Amy Cuddy on the effectiveness of power posing has been viewed over 45 million times and is near the top of the list of the most popular TED talks of all time. BAD HABITS The
“bikini” at the centre of the crisis refers to the way researchers collect and analyze data and report their results. Many important details and decisions are often concealed. When carrying
out experiments, researchers make decisions about how much data to collect, whether some observations should be excluded from the analysis and what controls, if any, should be included in
analyses. After the data has been collected, researchers have additional, undisclosed, leeway. They may “torture the data” until it reaches statistical significance (a cut-off that suggests
the real effect may not be zero), a practice called “p-hacking.” Or they may engage in the practice of “HARKing,” short for “hypothesizing after results are known.” Creating a hypothesis to
confirm a result that has already been found makes it easier to satisfy journal reviewers and editors who are interested in publishing statistically significant results. In academia, where
researchers are often under pressure to “publish or perish” to advance their careers and win grants, amassing publications is the route to success. All told, this undisclosed flexibility can
lead to extremely high rates of false positive results. A false positive is essentially claiming there is an effect when there isn’t one. An example would be concluding that standing up
straight increases testosterone levels, when it doesn’t. A NEW RESEARCH CULTURE Despite all the upheaval, psychology’s replication crisis may have a silver lining. In a few short years,
researchers have proposed many ideas and recommendations for reforming research with the goal of improvement. Journals and granting agencies are demanding more from authors with respect to
openness and transparency. There are accessible online repositories, such as Github, the Open Science Framework and OpenDOAR, that allow researchers to share their raw materials, exact
protocols, scripts, data, code, etc. with anyone who has an internet connection. The aim is to essentially have nothing concealed in the scientific process. Some journals, such as
_Psychological Science_, and recently American Psychological Association journals are encouraging authors to store their data and code in these repositories and to disclose details about
data collection decisions before submitting a manuscript for peer review. Researchers can also preregister their hypotheses. But something has been missing. THE MISSING LINK While
psychological science has been moving toward more open and transparent methods, graduate student training has been largely left out of discussions. Many of the practices that created the
crisis are embedded in our research culture: We do things a certain way because we have always done things this way and other people do too. Much of this culture is assimilated when
researchers are in graduate school. To sustain and maintain the momentum of positive change, it is important for graduate education to keep up with changes in the field. If training fails to
keep up, graduate students may leave programs with antiquated ideas and practices. These ideas and practices can proliferate as students become faculty members, start their own labs and
train graduate students in the same manner they were taught. Part of educating students is ensuring they are aware of the changing cultural landscape, and then explicitly teaching them to
follow open and transparent research practices and avoid bad habits. FINDING THE LIGHT In our department at the University of Guelph, a group of methodologically minded faculty have
recognized the importance of tackling this problem head on. Our goal is to create positive change and take steps to avoid history repeating itself with the next generation of researchers. We
created “Statistical methods in theses: Guidelines and explanations” to help students when conducting their thesis research. Students can work through the guidelines with their advisors,
allowing them to make better decisions in the planning stages of their research projects. The document’s rather humble sounding purpose belies an unintended provocative side. The guidelines
identify questionable research practices — to provide explanations and advice for students who wish to follow open and transparent research practices. Because some of the questionable
practices it identifies may be standard, previously unquestioned — and sometimes taught — procedures, the document has the potential to be viewed, by some, as extreme. Culture is not
something that can be changed overnight. But with explicit efforts to cultivate a new research culture, change can be targeted and purposeful. This crisis in psychology makes me think about
a line in John Milton’s epic poem, _Paradise Lost_: “Long is the way and hard, that out of Hell leads up to Light.” By acting on the crisis, psychology has embarked upon its symbolic journey
back to “light.” It will be current and future graduate students that will decide how, and where, the journey ends.