
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
ABSTRACT Alpha rhythms (9–11 Hz) are a dominant feature of EEG recordings, particularly over occipital cortex on cessation of a visual stimulation. Little is known about underlying
neocortical mechanisms so here we constructed alpha rhythm models that follow cessation of cortical stimulation. The rhythm manifests following a period of gamma frequency activity in local
V1 networks in layer 4. It associates with network level bias of excitatory synaptic activity in favour of NMDA- rather than AMPA-mediated signalling and reorganisation of synaptic
inhibition in favour of fast GABAA receptor-mediated events. At the cellular level the alpha rhythm depended upon the generation of layer 4 pyramidal neuron dendritic bursting mediated
primarily by PPDA-sensitive NR2C/D-containing NMDA receptors, which lack the magnesium-dependent open channel block. Subthreshold potassium conductances are also critical. The rhythm
dynamically filters outputs from sensory relay neurons (stellate neurons in layer 4) such that they become temporally uncoupled from downstream population activity. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING
VIEWED BY OTHERS PARABOLIC AVALANCHE SCALING IN THE SYNCHRONIZATION OF CORTICAL CELL ASSEMBLIES Article Open access 03 May 2023 SPONTANEOUS VARIABILITY IN GAMMA DYNAMICS DESCRIBED BY A
DAMPED HARMONIC OSCILLATOR DRIVEN BY NOISE Article Open access 19 April 2022 SHARP-WAVE RIPPLE DOUBLETS INDUCE COMPLEX DENDRITIC SPIKES IN PARVALBUMIN INTERNEURONS IN VIVO Article Open
access 07 November 2022 INTRODUCTION For most classical EEG/MEG frequency bands, the underlying biological processes are well understood1,2. This is not the case for the EEG alpha rhythm
(8–12 Hz). Despite being the earliest brain rhythm recorded in human3, we are still a long way from a complete definition of its underlying mechanisms. Consequently, any mechanistic link
between the presence of alpha rhythms and their many task-related cognitive functions remains uncertain. Of the many different manifestations of the alpha rhythm, the classical ‘eyes-closed’
occipital EEG alpha rhythm, is the most obvious example of both the phenomenon itself and its relation to the sensory stream. This type of alpha rhythm has been proposed to function as a
facet of short-term memory, countering the physiological need for eye-blinks (lacunae in the visual input stream) via the short-term maintenance of a visual scene in cortical code4. Further
examples of the rhythm’s role in short-term memory performance include: Alpha-rhythm power and coherence increases with memory load during retention tasks5 and memory retrieval6,7. In
addition, feature extraction in visual memory tasks directly correlates with alpha-rhythm modulation8. From these and many other precedents, alpha rhythms appear to maintain features of past
sensory events in memory for comparison with features of present events9. In addition, alpha rhythms may also selectively inhibit components of de novo sensory representations in cortex10,
with many studies demonstrating a role for alpha rhythms in selective attention11. Elevated alpha power is associated with presentation of task-irrelevant and distracting stimuli12 and, in
turn, with reduced firing rates in generating regions13. This role, and the above mnemonic role, may be related given the hypothesis linking active ‘selection’ of present sensory events to
memory of past experiences10,14. What is needed to help provide a mechanistic understanding of the above phenomena is a manipulable, biologically valid set of experimental and computational
models of the neocortical alpha rhythm. Such models exist for thalamus, where neurons generate broadband theta–alpha frequency oscillations when tonically depolarised15. This thalamic rhythm
arises in single neurons coupled by gap junctions, generating a population rhythm that has been modelled computationally in some detail16. However, in vivo EEG alpha rhythms are dominated
by a neocortical generator17,18 and occur over a much narrower frequency range. Neocortical alpha may in part arise from transient potentiation of thalamic inputs to layer 5 pyramids19.
These neurons do appear to have both intrinsic and excitatory network resonance at alpha–theta frequencies, but disinhibition was required to see this. In contrast, gap junctionally
connected low-threshold spiking (LTS) inhibitory interneurons in neocortex form a network capable of generating outputs at alpha–theta frequency20. None of these proposed mechanisms
completely reproduces the narrow band of alpha that is readily recordable over human occipital cortex, its pharmacological properties, nor its association with memory and attention. Here we
use experiment and biologically realistic computational simulations to characterise a novel, physiologically relevant model of the post visual stimulus, ‘eyes-closed’ rhythm in visual
cortex3. The model predicts that alpha rhythms may manifest on reduction of the strength of fast synaptic excitation following a period of pharmacologically stimulated gamma-rhythm
generation in primary visual cortex. In this case, alpha rhythms arise from layer 4 pyramidal neurons in a recurrently connected local network under conditions of enhanced NMDA
receptor-mediated activity and reduced AMPA receptor traffic. The transformation of frequency from gamma rhythms to alpha rhythms occurs via generation of overt, population-synchronous
bursting originating via NMDA spiking in dendrites on reduction of slow synaptic inhibition. This imbalance in local network inhibition and excitation, coupled with strong projection of the
L4 pyramid alpha rhythm to infra- and supragranular layers, dynamically filters layer 4 stellate neuron outputs, occluding them from influencing local downstream within-column neocortical
targets. RESULTS ALPHA RHYTHMS MANIFEST AS ‘REBOUND’ FOLLOWING GAMMA RHYTHMS To examine the generation of alpha rhythms following periods of primary visual cortical stimulation, we used two
experimental models. Firstly, to capture the time course of alpha-rhythm generation, glutamate (1 mM, 50–70 nl) was pressure ejected onto the main sensory input layer in V1 (L4) in
drug-naive coronal slices containing visual cortex. Responses were robust, lasting 2–11 s (median 4 s, _n_ = 12) and manifest as dual-gamma rhythms (e.g. see ref. 1) with modal peak
frequencies of 26 ± 4 and 61 ± 9 Hz. Repeating glutamate applications in slices bathed in 10 μM carbachol to mimic waking cholinergic neuromodulator state also generated transient epochs of
dual-gamma rhythms (median duration 7 s (IQR 4–12 s), frequencies 23 ± 3 and 55 ± 6 Hz, _n_ = 14). In the presence of carbachol, the gamma rhythms rapidly transitioned to an alpha rhythm (9
± 2 Hz, _n_ = 14) lasting over 20 s (Fig. 1a). The transience of the alpha rhythm generated in this manner, coupled with the mechanical instability induced by pressure ejection made
intracellular recordings difficult so we sought to extract the main features of these data and generate an ‘all-pharmacological’ experimental model with more persistent alpha rhythms to
facilitate mechanistic examination. Initial cortical excitation was induced by bath application of kainate (400 nM), which also produced dual-gamma-frequency spectral peaks in the local
field potential (LFP, Fig. 1B). This initial activation of fast glutamate receptor-mediated excitation was then reduced by low concentrations of the mixed antagonist NBQX. This abolished
high-gamma-frequency activity and significantly reduced low-gamma-frequency activity (power 20–30 Hz, 42 ± 6 μV2 to 7 ± 3 μV2, _P_ < 0.001, Fig. 1b). No alpha-frequency activity was seen
using this procedure. Mimicking the inhibitory effects of cholinergic neuromodulation on the hyperpolarisation-activated current _I_h by application of DK-AH269 (10 μM) did generate an overt
alpha rhythm in the LFP following reduction of fast glutamate receptor signalling with NBQX (power 70 ± 23 μV2, frequency 9 ± 1 Hz, _n_ = 68, Fig. 1b). Power in the alpha band remained
stable for over 180 min using these manipulations but did fade to undetectable levels in spectra by 2 h. In addition, the alpha rhythm generated in the above manner could be readily
abolished by re-establishing external excitatory drive—further glutamate ejections or removal of NBQX depending on the model used. ALPHA RHYTHMS ASSOCIATE WITH ACTIVITY IN V1 L4 Using the
model illustrated in Fig. 1b, we explored the origin of the rebound alpha rhythm in more detail. LFP recordings revealed strong alpha rhythms in V1 layers 2–6 (Fig. 2a) with mean power
largest in layer 4. Multi-unit activity was more spatially restricted by cortical layer, with a clear peak in spiking also seen in layer 4 (Fig. 2b). Current source density (CSD) analysis
showed a complex pattern of events on each alpha period. Average CSD profiles (_n_ = 20 consecutive periods, _N_ = 5 slices) revealed a primary source-sink pairing across L4 with additional
source/sink transitions in deep L5 (Fig. 2c, see also ref. 18). In vivo studies have shown alpha rhythms in most visual cortical subregions18, so we examined the regional extent of the
rhythm in the neocortex-only, in vitro slice model used here. Electrodes placed in L4 (or equivalent depth) across the coronal slice revealed the vast majority of alpha power resided in the
medial portion of V1 (Fig. 2d), with activity in adjacent secondary visual areas and retrosplenial cortex almost zero. We next examined whether the alpha rhythm generated by the
‘all-pharmacological’ experiments shown here was dependent on the initial period of V1 excitation generated by kainate or simply a direct product of the drugs sequentially applied.
Concurrent bath application of kainate, NBQX and DK-AH269 to a previously drug-naive slice failed to generate any rhythmic spectral components in the L4 LFP (Fig. 3a, _n_ = 5) suggesting the
initial gamma rhythm was required for subsequent alpha rhythms to manifest. We next used the inherent variability of high- and low-frequency gamma rhythms generated by the slices and
correlated the power of each of these when they were manifest alone (i.e. only high-gamma or only low-gamma) to subsequent alpha-rhythm power. Alpha-rhythm power was strongly correlated with
the power of prior low-gamma rhythms across the dataset (_R_2 = 0.84), but no correlation was seen for high-frequency gamma rhythms (_R_2 = 0.11, Fig. 3b). ALPHA RHYTHMS ASSOCIATE WITH
COMPLEX PATTERNS OF INHIBITION Rhythms in cortex have been proposed to all be primarily inhibition-based although there are several exceptions21, and synaptic inhibition is thought to
underlie the functional inhibition associated with the alpha rhythms22 (but see also ref. 23). Therefore, to investigate the cellular mechanisms underlying the L4 V1 alpha rhythm, we first
examined the profile of synaptic inhibition. We identified two different types of fast-spiking neurons with cell bodies in V1 L4. Both could generate spike rates over 200 Hz on somatic
depolarisation. One (FS type 1, _n_ = 5) showed large, brief afterhyperpolarisations (AHP) and no late afterhyperpolarisation on cessation of spiking. The other (FS type 2, _n_ = 4) showed
smaller, more prolonged post-spike AHPs and a clear, late AHP component (Fig. 4a). Behaviour of these two FS neurons differed in their output profiles both during the initial gamma rhythm on
kainate-only excitation and particularly during subsequent alpha rhythms. Both FS neurons generated spikes on each period of the initial gamma rhythm, but FS1 neurons often generated spike
doublets. During the LFP alpha rhythm, FS1 neurons followed the field alpha rhythm with 2–4 spikes per period. However, during this alpha rhythm, FS2 neurons remained quiescent despite
receiving alpha-frequency compound excitatory inputs (Fig. 4a). This loss of output from one type of FS neuron was accompanied by the loss of one type of inhibitory postsynaptic potential
(IPSP) in stellate neurons in V1 L4 (Fig. 4b). The decay times of IPSPs were examined for each of the two main principal neuron types in V1 L4—small pyramids and stellate neurons—and
arranged by decay time to examine the incidence of IPSPs with different kinetics during the initial gamma period and subsequent alpha rhythms (_n_ = 684 IPSPs from _N_ = 8 stellate neurons,
_n_ = 890 IPSPs from _N_ = 10 pyramids). During the initial gamma rhythm, stellate neuron synaptic inhibition was dominated by IPSPs with mean decay time of 19 ± 6 ms, whereas pyramid IPSPs
had a mean decay time of 8 ± 2 ms. Despite this difference both principal neuron subtypes received the same mean duration of synaptic inhibition per gamma period when defined as the overall
duration of the compound IPSP at 50% maximum amplitude (stellate 19 ± 2 ms, pyramid 17 ± 3 ms, example compound IPSPs from multiple neurons, aligned to peak IPSC events, shown in Fig. 4c.
During the subsequent alpha rhythm, stellate neuron individual IPSP decay time incidences now matched those for pyramidal cells (Fig. 4b). However, the duration of compound inhibitory events
per alpha period diverged markedly (Fig. 4c). Stellate neuron inhibition consisted entirely of small, single, brief events per period (duration 6 ± 1 ms), whereas pyramid compound IPSP
inhibition times were prolonged to 45 ± 7 ms. ALPHA RHYTHMS ARISE FROM L4 PYRAMIDAL DENDRITIC BURSTING The above profile of synaptic inhibition during the alpha rhythm (with different IPSP
kinetics in stellate neurons and L4 pyramidal neurons) was not consistent with the classical idea of oscillation period being set by IPSPs, for example, as seen for gamma rhythms24. There
was no correspondence between the prolonged period length and slower individual IPSP decay times when comparing gamma and alpha rhythms. Instead, the data suggested a specific pattern of
activation of interneurons feeding back onto pyramids, but with rhythm periodicity set by the pyramidal neurons themselves, i.e. no prolongation of the decay kinetics was seen accompanying
the prolonged period length. Instead, the profile of synaptic inhibition merely followed the change in output from pyramids. To explore this further, we examined the output profile of
pyramids during the alpha rhythm in more detail and sought to computationally model the core mechanisms sufficient for its generation. Both stellate and pyramidal principal cells generated
single spikes on each period of the initial gamma rhythm (spike probability per gamma period 0.84 (pyramid), 0.52 (stellate)). However, during the rebound alpha rhythm spike numbers per
alpha period in pyramids increased to 2.4 ± 0.6 (_n_ = 100 periods, _N_ = 8 pyramidal cells). The overall spike frequency in these cells was unchanged when comparing gamma rhythms to alpha
rhythms (gamma 20 ± 3 Hz, alpha 23 ± 4 Hz, _P_ > 0.05). This occurred despite a significant net hyperpolarisation of L4 pyramid somata when comparing the initial gamma rhythm with the
subsequent alpha rhythm. During the gamma rhythm, L4 somatic recordings revealed a mean membrane potential of −62 ± 2 mV, whereas on manifestation of the LFP alpha rhythm, these neurons
hyperpolarised to −69 ± 2 mV (_P_ < 0.05, _n_ = 8 somatic recordings). Analysis of the phase relationship between L4 pyramid spikes, and the LFP alpha rhythm revealed a highly variable
pattern of somatic spike-field timing (Fig. 5a). Dual maxima in spike incidence were seen with the majority of spikes clustering around the peak negativity in the LFP alpha rhythm, but with
an additional cluster of spikes being generated 30–50 ms later. This complex profile of variable, multiple spiking was phase-locked to the LFP alpha rhythm suggesting a role for outputs from
L4 pyramids in alpha-rhythm generation. However, somatic recordings alone yielded few concrete clues as to the origin of this peculiar spike profile. We therefore examined recordings from
apical dendrites of L4 pyramids. The timing of this second cluster of spikes seen during alpha rhythms but not the initial gamma rhythms coincided with generation of a broad dendritic
depolarisation (Fig. 5b). The variability in somatic action potential generation appeared to correlate with the highly variable nature of these dendritic bursts (amplitude 15 (7–26) mV,
half-width 38 (10–52) ms, _n_ = 10 periods, _N_ = 10 dendrites). These dendritic bursts were also accompanied by a net maximal hyperpolarisation of dendrites when comparing gamma conditions
to alpha-rhythm conditions (−65 ± 3 mV (gamma), −75 ± 5 mV (alpha), _P_ < 0.05, _n_ = 5 dendritic recordings). To explore further, the mechanisms linking the unusual temporal profile of
somatic spike generation to bursting in hyperpolarised dendrites, we constructed multicompartment, computational models of L4 pyramidal cells (Methods). The models with active dendrites
reproduced the observed complex patterns of somatic and dendritic electrogenesis with high fidelity (Fig. 5a, b, right panel), with overt dendritic burst spiking initiated by
back-propagating somatic action potentials and sustained by dendritic NMDA receptor activation. The initial cluster of spikes per period (with interspike intervals >5 ms, Fig. 5a, lower
panel) were predicted to arise from a combination of gap junction-mediated neuronal coupling (when modelled) and a strong afterdepolarisation mediated by persistent sodium channels. Multiple
mechanisms were found to potentially underlie the later dendritic burst behaviour, including voltage-operated calcium channels and NMDA receptor-mediated dendritic bursts (Discussion). We
used these predictions to guide further experiments to investigate underlying mechanisms and relate these to generation of the LFP alpha rhythm. None of the voltage-operated calcium channel
blockers used significantly affected the post-gamma rhythm alpha frequency local field potential (_P_ > 0.05, _n_ = 5, Fig. 6a). However, general blockade of NMDA receptor-mediated events
with the broad-spectrum antagonist R-CPP almost completely abolished the field alpha rhythm (_P_ < 0.05, _n_ = 5). Perhaps not surprisingly, given the hyperpolarised state of dendrites
during the alpha rhythm (see above), the main subunits of the NMDA receptors involved in this effect were of the NR2C/D subtype. This was revealed by significant reduction in field alpha
power with the NR2C/D-preferring antagonist PPDA (_P_ < 0.05, Fig. 6a). This antagonism of the field alpha rhythm with PPDA was accompanied by an abolition of the broad, late dendritic
bursts giving rise to the second cluster of action potentials seen on most alpha periods (Fig. 6b). Some involvement of NMDA receptors containing the NR2B subunit was also seen, but no
involvement of receptors containing the main adult form of NMDA receptors (containing the NR2A subunit) was detected. In addition, experiments using the EAG (ether-a-go-go) potassium channel
blocker terfenadine demonstrated a significant involvement of these subthreshold-activated channels in maintaining the field alpha rhythm (_P_ < 0.05, Fig. 6a). This latter observation
also reinforced the data suggesting a core role of L4 pyramidal cells in generating the alpha rhythm. All pyramidal cells recorded (_n_ = 10) and none of the stellate cells examined (_n_ =
12) were found to be immunopositive for the EAG channel Kv10.2 (Fig. 6c). ‘FUNCTIONAL INHIBITION’ DURING ALPHA RHYTHMS IS DYNAMIC The alpha rhythm is thought to functionally inhibit cortical
responses to unattended components of sensory input10. This functional inhibition has been directly linked to periodic, ‘pulsed’ alpha-frequency synaptic inhibition22,25. These authors
consider the inhibition to be sinusoidal but we found no evidence of this here. However, evidence supporting the general concept of mismatched inhibition time in neurons and networks serving
to filter-out certain signals was seen when comparing the profile of spike generation in L4 principal neurons (stellate and pyramid) and their compound inhibitory inputs (Fig. 4). We
therefore investigated the temporal pattern of neuronal spiking in layers 2–5 to establish whether the alpha rhythm studied here was capable of such functional inhibition both within and
beyond L4. First, we explored principal neuron spike timing within L4. During the initial gamma rhythm both pyramids and stellate neurons generated single spikes on most periods (see above
for incidences and frequencies). These spikes were strongly phase-locked to the gamma field potential maxima for both principal cell subtypes (Fig. 7a). This parity between neuron subtypes
was lost on manifestation of the field alpha rhythm. Overall, spike rates were not significantly changed when comparing frequencies during gamma and alpha rhythms in pyramidal cells (see
above). However, stellate neuron spike rates decreased markedly when comparing the initial gamma rhythm and the subsequent alpha rhythm (15 ± 2 Hz (gamma), 8 ± 1 Hz (alpha), _P_ < 0.05),
this owing to the generation of only single spikes per alpha period in contrast to the complex, variable multiple spiking seen for pyramidal cells (cf. Fig. 5a). In addition, the phase
locking of stellate neuron spikes to the LFP rhythm was abolished on transition from gamma to alpha rhythms (Fig. 7a vs. Fig. 7b). This overt failure of spike-field temporal organisation was
reproduced by the computational model (Fig. 7c) suggesting a possible role for mismatched inhibition (see above) secondary to differential expression of dendritic bursting—no somatic
multiple spikes or late afterdepolarisations per alpha period were seen in any stellate neuron studied (_n_ = 12). In this model, this may represent indirect evidence for lack of the
dendritic bursts seen in pyramids. The above temporal uncoupling of stellate neuron outputs from pyramidal neuron outputs had consequences for stellate neuron signals in the wider columnar
network. Comparing the spike times of L4 stellate and pyramidal cells to those from L2/3 regular spiking cells and L5 intrinsically bursting cells revealed strong temporal coupling during
the initial gamma rhythm (Fig. 8a). During subsequent alpha rhythms, the spike timing of these supra- and infragranular neurons continued to be co-modulated with L4 pyramids and thus the
field alpha rhythm. However, stellate neuronal spiking failed to demonstrate temporal coupling to both L2/3 and L5 neurons in the wider network (Fig. 8b) suggesting that failure to
participate in the L4 pyramid-mediated alpha rhythm effectively temporally filtered stellate neuron outputs out of downstream population activity26. DISCUSSION The experimental data and
computational simulations presented here suggest that the classical, ‘striate’ visual alpha rhythm is manifest in local circuits of V1 following periods of slow gamma-frequency activity. The
model investigated here had a number of similarities with the phenomenon as studied non-invasively in human subjects: it required a prior period of excitatory (sensory) activity; it was
time-limited, fading as time from this prior excitation increased; it was abolished on subsequent re-establishment of excitation (sensory input); it was sensitive to NMDA receptor
blockade27; its primary generation mechanism appeared to be largely synaptic inhibition-independent23; it was reduced by the EAG blocker terfenadine28. However, precedents for multiple
mechanisms underlying a single spectral EEG frequency exist. For example, the beta2-frequency band can be generated in parietal cortex via glutamatergic receptor activation with periodicity
mechanisms entirely intrinsic to layer 5 pyramids21, but a spectrally identical rhythm in auditory cortex induced by cholinergic neuromodulation required synaptic inhibition29. Thus, whether
the model presented here is mechanistically related to the posterior, extrastiate alpha rhythm, the thalamic alpha rhythm or alpha frequencies manifest in frontal cortices remains to be
elucidated. The most obvious correlation of the generation of the form of alpha rhythm studied here in vitro was the presence of overt dendritic bursting in layer 4 pyramids (Fig. 5b,
summary Fig. 9). This was observed concurrently with an increase in the NMDA/AMPA ratio of excitatory synaptic signalling and an accompanying failure to recruit some interneuron
subpopulations generating postsynaptic inhibitory events with decay constants ca. 20 ms. The rhythm was dependent upon the presence of a subthreshold, non-inactivating potassium conductance
of the EAG subtype. Such overt changes in local circuit frequency are precedented for burst generation in other principal cell types: Recruitment of layer 5 intrinsically bursting pyramids
into a gamma rhythm is abolished in favour of beta2-frequency activity via burst generation in gap junctionally connected axons21 and further modified in this cell type to produce
delta-frequency rhythms on generation of dendritic bursts30. However, in the relatively under-studied L4 pyramid considered here, the network conditions favouring burst generation produced
an alpha rhythm, with the profile of dendritic bursting being somewhat different. Two main forms of dendritic bursting have been characterised in L5 pyramids. Complex bursts from main
branches of pyramid apical dendrites occur via voltage-operated calcium channel conductances, whereas bursts mediated by NMDA receptors are seen on finer dendritic processes31. Both have a
role in synaptic integration and together have been shown to powerfully influence sensory processing32,33. The bursts seen in L4 pyramid recordings during the alpha rhythm appeared to be
subtly different from L5 pyramid behaviour. Although non-synaptic dendritic bursting was possible in L4 pyramids (Fig. 5b), no involvement of individual voltage-operated calcium channel
subtypes were observed to contribute to the alpha rhythm (Fig. 6a). This may indicate the involvement of a complex admixture of channel subtypes or a dominant involvement of NMDA spikes.
Both the bursts and the alpha rhythm were dependent on blockade of hyperpolarisation-activated conductance _I_h. The latter intrinsic conductance state is essential for generating dendritic
NMDA bursts33 suggesting the change in dendritic electrogenesis essential for alpha-rhythm generation was almost entirely mediated by NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic excitation, a factor
that may also contribute to the strong local synchrony inherent in the LFP alpha-rhythm recordings. The most likely source of this synaptic excitation is recurrent connectivity within L4
where principal cells are strongly and densely interconnected. The nature of this connectivity appears highly suited to the relatively hyperpolarised, low external excitation state during
which alpha rhythms are observed: Perhaps uniquely in neocortex, there is a strong NR2C-dependent component to L4 recurrent connectivity34, and NMDARs containing this subunit are relatively
insensitive to the Mg2+-mediated open channel NMDA block that normally limits conductance through this channel at the hyperpolarised membrane potentials associated with the alpha rhythm
here35. The slow kinetics of NR2C-containing NMDARs suggest they do not, alone, give rise to the dendritic bursts seen during the alpha rhythm. However, blockade of NR2B-containing NMDARs
also significantly attenuated the alpha rhythm, suggesting the activation of non-open channel blocked NMDARs may have served to prime dendrites to burst via conventional NMDARs on back
propagation of sodium spikes seen to initiate events on each alpha period (Fig. 6b). Although the NMDA:AMPA ratio was modified experimentally in the present study, it is known to be highly
modifiable in a use-dependent manner. Desynchronisation of excitatory synaptic activity as seen here for stellate neurons and pyramids can enhance the ratio suggesting a feedforward
mechanism that may serve to stabilise alpha rhythms36. NMDA receptors in general appear to be ca. 100-fold more sensitive to glutamate than AMPA receptors37, a property, which would directly
translate into enhanced NMDA:AMPA signalling if no open channel block were present, i.e. in the precedented NR2C/D-containing recurrent excitatory synapses between L4 principal cells (see
above). This bias towards NMDAR-mediated signalling also suggests the dependence of the experimental models on prior low-gamma activity may involve direct NMDAR plasticity38 with
intracellular calcium elevations required arising from the calcium elevations seen in dendrites following 20–40 Hz stimulation39. The apparent change in synaptic excitation on transition
from gamma- to alpha-frequency may also explain, in part, the accompanying alterations in synaptic inhibition profile seen. Multiple subtypes of inhibitory interneuron have been reported in
V1 L4 including those immunopositive for parvalbumin, calretinin, neuropeptide Y and somatostatin40. Multiple cytoarchitectonic subtypes are also seen, including one perhaps unique to L4—the
‘clutch’ cell41. Here we found two subtypes with cell bodies within L4 and a clear dual profile of postsynaptic inhibitory kinetics (Fig. 4b). The slower inhibitory events associated with
the low-gamma rhythm42 were exclusively seen in stellate neuron somata. Silencing of this IPSP component accompanied the generation of the alpha rhythm, suggesting their cellular origin may
involve interneurons with fewer NMDAR subunits than conventional gamma-generating, parvalbumin-immunopositive cells43. In general, parvalbumin positive cells may have a strong NR2C/D subunit
expression profile32 and, in addition, the converse of the situation responsible for alpha-rhythm generation—blocking AMPA receptors and preserving NMDAR-mediated signalling—switches
network inhibition away from parvalbumin-immunopositive cells and modifies the gamma rhythm44. In addition, a change in inhibition profile at the level of proximal dendritic compartments—not
conventional targets for parvalbumin positive interneurons—may also contribute to both the generation of pyramidal neuron bursts and their expression as excitatory events in perisomatic
compartments as seen here. The above data suggest a role for synaptic inhibition in generating the alpha rhythm. However, whatever role the change in inhibition profile has it is clearly
different from inhibition-based rhythms where the periodicity is critically dependent on inhibition decay kinetics (e.g. the gamma and primary sensory beta2 rhythms): The computational model
can produce an alpha oscillation using an IPSP source with appropriate decay, but this does not generate the observed L4 pyramid firing pattern. The alpha periodicity appeared inherent in
the dendritic electrogenesis. The 10 Hz rhythmicity was robust and hard to modify experimentally. Three factors may contribute to the observation that the alpha rhythm is insensitive to
drugs that prolong inhibitory synaptic events23. Firstly, the change in profile of FS1 spiking and fast synaptic inhibition (Fig. 4) strongly suggested inhibition was dependent on the
profile of excitation received by interneurons. Studies on the gamma rhythm have shown that prolonging decay times for inhibition has much less of an effect in such situations than those
involving recurrent inhibition alone (‘ING’ vs ‘PING’ rhythms24). In other words, it is the kinetics of pyramidal dendritic electrogenesis, leading to the burst seen, that set the
periodicity of the alpha rhythm and not the kinetics of synaptic inhibition. Secondly, the compound nature of the inhibition received by L4 pyramids (‘bursts’ of fast-decaying IPSPs, Fig.
4c) suggests that changes in individual IPSP decay times would have much less effect on overall duration of inhibition. The data here showed a decrease in overall pyramidal neuron inhibitory
input kinetics when comparing gamma to alpha rhythms (Figs. 4 and 9), but overall inhibition times increased in line with the spread of additional pyramid outputs secondary to dendritic
bursting. Finally, even these longer, compound inhibitory events in pyramidal cells during alpha rhythms only account for ca. 50% of each alpha period. If inhibition time alone was
responsible for setting the alpha periodicity, one would expect a beta-frequency rhythm from these data. In addition to synaptic inhibition, the present study found that intrinsic inhibition
was also vital for alpha-rhythm generation—that is, conductances counter to action potential generation inherent in individual neuron membranes. The critical role for EAG-like channels
pointed to a unique mechanism in neocortex: EAGs—particular EAG2 (Kv10.2)—are expressed in a highly laminar fashion, being almost exclusively expressed by a subset of L4 neurons45. Many
principal neuron subtypes (and low threshold spiking interneurons) alter their action potential output profiles on the basis of membrane potential. Depolarised membrane potentials favour
regular, single spiking (as seen here during gamma rhythms), whereas hyperpolarised potentials facilitate burst discharges46, as seen here during the alpha rhythm. The role of this
subthreshold potassium conductance may therefore simply reflect a maintenance of hyperpolarised membrane potentials during the alpha rhythm to promote the observed, underlying burst
discharges. The selective role for L4 pyramids, in contrast to stellate cells, seen in the present work indicates a robust mechanism for the ‘functional inhibition’ associated with the alpha
rhythm. Alpha rhythms can be seen to actively suppress stimulus responses47,48. Elevated alpha power is associated with a range of competing, task-irrelevant and distracting stimuli49.
However, this apparent inhibition of a task-irrelevant area almost certainly does not correspond, at least locally, to conventional synaptic inhibition, leading researchers to propose a
dynamic form of inhibition related to timing of neural events50. This attractive hypothesis still relies, indirectly on synaptic inhibition. In the reduced preparation used here a clear
uncoupling of stellate neuron outputs from the local L4 population dynamics was seen (Fig. 7), and this was accompanied by overt imbalance in synaptic inhibition received by stellate neurons
and pyramids (Fig. 4c). However, further, downstream consequences of this uncoupling in L4 may have nothing to do with synaptic inhibition. The L4 pyramid network-mediated alpha rhythm was
powerfully projected to both infra- and supragranular layers. Consequently, the timing of stellate neuron outputs was mismatched to on-going L2/3 and L5 activity. Such mismatches generate a
powerful filter, resulting in occlusion of the non-phase-locked component26. This ‘filter’ may have more widespread influence beyond the downstream signal from local columnal stellate cells.
Stellate cells develop in an experience-dependent manner from the initial purely pyramidal population of layer 4 principal cells51. This process involves reorganisation of dendritic arbours
but also associates with a loss of long-range horizontal projections onto distal layer 4 principal cell oblique dendrites52, suggesting that the alpha rhythm generated by local circuits
here may influence broad swathes of the primary visual cortical mantle. In summary, the experimental and computational models explored here suggested a lamina- and principal neuron
subtype-specific origin for the visual alpha rhythm. The unique profile of outputs from these neurons during the alpha rhythm served to powerfully occlude outputs from the local primary
sensory input processing neurons—L4 stellates—from on-going cortical activity via temporal filtering. The dependence on EAG-like potassium conductances, coupled with the near ubiquitous
presence of this neuron subtype marker in mid-layers of neocortex, even regions not classically assigned a layer 445, suggests similar mechanisms may underlie other forms of the alpha rhythm
such as the auditory ‘tau’ rhythm, the somatosensory ‘mu’ rhythm and the frontal alpha rhythm associated with resting state53. In addition, the demonstration of dependence on EAG-like
conductances and NMDA receptor subtypes may focus future genetic studies on the inter-subject diversity, and monozygotic twin similarity, of alpha-rhythm power and frequency and perhaps also
aid further mechanistic understanding of neurological disorders associated with altered alpha-rhythm generation. METHODS BASIC ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY Coronal sections, 450-μm-thick, containing
primary visual cortex (V1) were prepared from brains of adult male Wistar rats (150–200 g) following cardiac perfusion with ice-cold buffered sucrose acsf (252 mM sucrose, 3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM
NaH2PO4, 24 mM NaHCO3, 2 mM MgSO4, 2 mM CaCl2, 10 mM glucose). All surgical procedures were in accordance with regulations of the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986 and the
University of York animal welfare committee. Slices were maintained at 33 °C at the interface between acsf (126 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 24 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM MgSO4, 1.2 mM CaCl2, 10
mM glucose) and warm, wetted 95% O2/5% CO2. Local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded using glass microelectrodes (resistance > 1 MΩ) filled with acsf, whereas intracellular recordings
used electrodes (50–90 MΩ) filled with 2 M KAc and 5% w/v biocytin. Electrode array data used for current source density (CSD) and multi-unit activity (MUA) were taken from 8 × 4 tungsten
arrays with 0.2 mm electrode separation (Tucker Davis Technologies). Laminar profiles were obtained by averaging data along each of the four columns of electrodes for each slice. CSD
analysis was performed using iCSD54. EXPERIMENTAL MODELS OF POST-EXCITATION ALPHA RHYTHMS Visual alpha rhythms are most prominent on termination of a period of visual sensory stimulation
during the wake state3. We first modelled this by mimicking the wake state with low-levels of cholinergic neuromodulation by bath application of 10 μM carbachol. On this baseline, glutamate
(1 mM, ca. 50 nl) was pressure ejected onto L4 of primary visual cortex (V1) to generate local network activity associated with visual processing. Although this model served to demonstrate
the rapid ‘rebound’ from gamma frequency, local population activity to a local alpha rhythm the mechanical disruption caused by pressure ejection countered the maintenance of stable
intracellular recordings. In addition, the resulting alpha rhythm lasted only 20–110 s (_n_ = 4) making pre- and post-drug manipulation experiments impossible during the alpha period. We
therefore switched to an ‘all-pharmacological’ model capturing the core elements involved: Initial excitation generating gamma rhythms with kainate (400 nM), reduced 30 min later by further
acsf application of 4 μM NBQX to reduce AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic excitation and DK-AH269 (10 μM, and an HCN channel blocker) to mimic the cellular effects of waking neuromodulatory
state induced by carbachol. Note, simply removing kainate from the bathing medium following excitation did not generate alpha rhythms. We considered this as a facet of the slow rate of
removal of the excitatory drive via kainate from the bathing medium, hence the use of a low concentration of antagonist (NBQX) to more rapidly decrease excitatory drive. The contributions of
intrinsic and synaptic conductances to the alpha rhythm were explored by bath application of selective blockers of voltage-operated calcium channels (Amlodipine (10 μM) L-channels, NNC
55-0396 (50 μM) T-channels, ω-conotoxin GIVA (50 nM) N-channels, ω-agatoxin TK (50 nM) P/Q-channels)), NMDA receptors (R-CPP (10 μM) NR1/all subtype-containing, PPDA (10 μM) for
NR2C/D-containing, TCN201 (10 μM) for NR2A-containing, CO101244 (10 μM), for NR2B-containing, ifenprodil (10 μM) for polyamine/presynaptic NMDARs including NR2B, GABAA receptors (gabazine, 1
μM), m-current (XE991, 1 μM) and subthreshold-activated and non-inactivating potassium channels (EAG2/Kv10.2, terfenadine, 10 μM). All drugs were obtained from Tocris or Sigma.
Intracellular recordings from neurons with somata in layer 4 were initially classified via intrinsic properties—spike rate, width and afterhyperpolarisation profile for principal cells and
interneurons, afterdepolarisation magnitude and spike profile per LFP period for different principal cells. Classification was validated post hoc via biocytin reconstruction (see below).
Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) were recorded by holding the impaled neuron compartment at −30 mV with injected current (+0.2 to +0.5 nA). Decay time was estimated using Axograph
software from the maximum voltage deflection down to 50% of return to baseline using IPSPs that expressed enough decay to see this (i.e. not those embedded as part of the highly-compound
events observed). IPSP duration—for all IPSPs including the compound events—was quantified as the total duration of the event at 50% maximal magnitude. Dendritic recordings were obtained
blind at the level of layer 3 (i.e. 100–300 μm from layer 4 neuron somata). Recordings were assumed as dendritic if they demonstrated both high input resistance and high capacitance compared
to somatic recordings. Validation, and identification of the cell subtype from which the recording was taken, was performed post hoc by examining the cytoarchitecture of the
biocytin-filled, recorded neuron (see below). Temporal inhibition (Fig. 8) was quantified by first constructing mean spike incidences for each neuron subtype relative to the concurrently
recorded LFP (60 s epochs pooled from _n_ = 4 neurons, one per slice). Spike incidences per neuron subtype were then pairwise cross correlated using the raw ‘xcov’ function in Matlab.
IMMUNOCYTOCHEMISTRY All recorded cells were filled with biocytin and processed for immunhistochemistry. After the recordings, slices were fixed in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PBS) containing 4%
paraformaldehyde at 4 °C overnight. After fixation, slices were gelatin embedded and re-sectioned at 50 μm thickness. Slices were then rinsed in PBS, followed by antigen retrieval with 0.01
M sodium citrate for 10 min, and pre-incubation with 10% horse serum in 0.1 M TBS for 2 h. Immunopositivity for KV10.2 was tested with a primary antibody raised in rabbit (1:1000, Alomone
Labs, Cat# APC-053, RRID: AB_2039935, rabbit, polyclonal). Further immunolabeling was used against ROR beta (1:1000, RORbeta (K-16); Santa Cruz, Cat#sc-21354, RRID: AB_2180298; goat,
polyclonal). The following secondary antibodies were used to visualise the immunoreactions: Alexa 488-conjugated donkey anti-rabbit (1:200, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Cat# R-37118), Alexa
647-conjugated donkey anti-goat IgG1 (1:200, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Cat# A-21447). Biocytin labelling was visualised with Alexa 647- conjugated streptavidin (1:200, Thermo Fisher
Scientific, Cat# S11225). High magnification fluorescent images were acquired with a fully automated Zeiss LSM 710 confocal microscope, using ×20 and ×63 objectives. COMPUTATIONAL MODELLING
For the present study, we adapted the code of our thalamocortical network models30,55, which contain populations of a number of different neuron types, each simulated with multi-conductance
multicompartment models. Here, neurons in deep neocortical layers (bursting and regular spiking (RS) pyramids and deep interneurons) were disconnected. Layer 4 pyramids, for the present
case, were represented with subroutines originally written for superficial (i.e. layer 2/3) regular spiking pyramidal cells, modified as described below. Thus, the present network model
consisted of small RS pyramids (240 of them), spiny stellate neurons (240), basket interneurons (90), axoaxonic interneurons (90), low threshold spiking (LTS) interneurons (90) and
neurogliaform interneurons (90). Synaptic connections involved excitatory “AMPA receptors” and “NMDA receptors”, as well as “GABAA receptors”, but not “GABAB receptors”. Axoaxonal gap
junctions were included in some, but not all, simulations (and not in the simulations illustrated here). Some of the structural details were as follows: each pyramid was synaptically excited
by 50 other (randomly chosen) pyramids, via AMPA and NMDA receptors. Critically, the NMDA conductance decay time constant was 15 ms vs. the usual 130 ms. A pyramidal neuron also received
inputs from 20 randomly chosen interneurons, of each type, and 20 spiny stellate neurons. Small bias currents (−0.15 to 0.045 nA) were applied to the soma and mid-apical compartments. Slow
AHP currents were virtually abolished and _I_h was completely blocked in pyramids (consistent with experiment). Persistent _g_Na was absent in pyramidal axons, but present in soma and
dendrites. The conductance of “_g_K(C)” was increased 2-fold, compared with previous code, to prevent excessive bursting. The conductance of T-type Ca2+ conductance varied amongst
simulations, but was found to not influence greatly the network behaviour (consistent with experiment). The density of high-threshold Ca2+ conductance (“_g_Ca(L)”) was larger than in earlier
superficial pyramid simulations, here reaching 0.6 mS/cm2 in distal apical dendrites; this helped to permit the observed dendritic bursts. A non-inactivating K+ conductance, called
“_g_K(M)” in the code was present, with kinetics described by Hodgkin–Huxley-like forward and backward rate functions: $$\alpha \left( {{V}} \right) = {\mathrm{1}}.{\mathrm{6}}/\left(
{{\mathrm{1}} + {\mathrm{exp}}\left( { - 0.0{\mathrm{72 }}\left( {{{V}}-{\mathrm{5}}.0} \right)} \right)} \right)$$ and $$\beta \left( {{V}} \right) = [({{V}} +
{\mathrm{8}}.{\mathrm{9}})/{\mathrm{5}}0.0]/[{\mathrm{exp}}(({{V}} + {\mathrm{8}}.{\mathrm{9}})/{\mathrm{5}})-1]$$ The NMDA conductance kinetics were simulated as before55, with a scaling
factor, a time-dependent term, and a voltage- and [Mg2+]-dependent term; the difference being that for pyramid/pyramid connections the decay time constant was 15 vs. 130 ms for other
connections between excitatory neurons. The previous models used evolved from prior models we have published using NMDA decay constants derived from hippocampal pyramidal cells. These are in
the order of 102 ms, whereas the nearest relevant precedent for visual L4 NMDA time constants is approximately 1/10th of this (ca. 20 ms34). Code was in Fortran in the mpi parallel
environment and was compiled with “mpxlf”. It was run on an IBM 7040-681 computer using the AIX operating system. The database consisted of over 200 network simulations, as well as
preliminary simulations of single pyramids. REPORTING SUMMARY Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article. DATA
AVAILABILITY Data that support the findings of this study (LFP, electrode array, intracellular and immunocytochemistry) are available, with metadata in ‘figshare’
(10.6084/m9.figshare.11762655, 10.6084/m9.figshare.11762640, 10.6084/m9.figshare.11762565, 10.6084/m9.figshare.11762508). Computational model code is available from ModelDB
(http://modeldb.yale.edu/263703). REFERENCES * Ainsworth, M. et al. Rates and rhythms: a synergistic view of frequency and temporal coding in neuronal networks. _Neuron_ 75, 572–583 (2012).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kopell, N., Kramer, M. A., Malerba, P. & Whittington, M. A. Are different rhythms good for different functions? _Front Hum. Neurosci._ 4, 187
(2010). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Berger, H. Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen. _Arch. f.ür. Psychiatr. und Nervenkrankheiten_ 87, 527–570 (1929). Article
Google Scholar * VanRullen, R. & Macdonald, J. S. Perceptual echoes at 10 Hz in the human brain. _Curr. Biol._ 22, 995–999 (2012). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Jensen, O.,
Kaiser, J. & Lachaux, J. P. Human gamma-frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. _Trends Neurosci._ 30, 317–324 (2007). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Palva,
S. & Palva, J. M. New vistas for alpha-frequency band oscillations. _Trends Neurosci._ 30, 150–158 (2007). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Klimesch, W. α-band oscillations,
attention, and controlled access to stored information. _Trends Cogn. Sci._ 16, 606–617 (2012). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P. & Hanslmayr,
S. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. _Brain Res. Rev._ 53, 63–88 (2007). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Zauner, A. et al. Alpha entrainment is responsible for the
attentional blink phenomenon. _Neuroimage_ 63, 674–686 (2012). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. _Annu Rev.
Neurosci._ 18, 193–222 (1995). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Foxe, J. J., Simpson, G. V. & Ahlfors, S. P. Parieto-occipital approximately 10 Hz activity reflects anticipatory
state of visual attention mechanisms. _Neuroreport_ 9, 3929–3933 (1998). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Sauseng, P. et al. Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory
capacity. _Curr. Biol._ 19, 1846–1852 (2009). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Haegens, S., Nácher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R. & Jensen, O. α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor
network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 108, 19377–19382 (2011). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google
Scholar * Knudsen, E. I. Fundamental components of attention. _Annu Rev. Neurosci._ 30, 57–78 (2007). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hughes, S. W. et al. Synchronized oscillations
at alpha and theta frequencies in the lateral geniculate nucleus. _Neuron_ 42, 253–268 (2004). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Vijayan, S. & Kopell, N. J. Thalamic model of awake
alpha oscillations and implications for stimulus processing. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 109, 18553–18558 (2012). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Lopes da Silva, F.
Neural mechanisms underlying brain waves: from neural membranes to networks. _Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol._ 79, 81–93 (1991). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bollimunta,
A., Chen, Y., Schroeder, C. E. & Ding, M. Neuronal mechanisms of cortical alpha oscillations in awake-behaving macaques. _J. Neurosci._ 28, 9976–9988 (2008). Article CAS PubMed PubMed
Central Google Scholar * Silva, L. R., Amitai, Y. & Connors, B. W. Intrinsic oscillations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal neurons. _Science_ 251, 432–435 (1991). Article
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Deans, M. R., Gibson, J. R., Sellitto, C., Connors, B. W. & Paul, D. L. Synchronous activity of inhibitory networks in neocortex requires electrical
synapses containing connexin36. _Neuron_ 31, 477–485 (2001). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Roopun, A. K. et al. A beta2-frequency (20-30 Hz) oscillation in nonsynaptic networks of
somatosensory cortex. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 103, 15646–15650 (2006). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Jensen, O. & Mazaheri, A. Shaping functional
architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. _Front. Hum. Neurosci._ 4, 186 (2010). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Lozano-Soldevilla, D., ter Huurne,
N., Cools, R. & Jensen, O. GABAergic modulation of visual gamma and alpha oscillations and its consequences for working memory performance. _Curr. Biol._ 24, 2878–2887 (2014). Article
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Whittington, M. A., Traub, R. D., Kopell, N., Ermentrout, B. & Buhl, E. H. Inhibition-based rhythms: experimental and mathematical observations on network
dynamics. _Int J. Psychophysiol._ 38, 315–336 (2000). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bonnefond, M., Kastner, S. & Jensen, O. Communication between brain areas based on nested
oscillations. _eNeuro_ 4, ENEURO.0153-16.2017 (2017). * Akam, T. & Kullmann, D. M. Oscillations and filtering networks support flexible routing of information. _Neuron_ 67, 308–320
(2010). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Vlisides, P. E. et al. Subanaesthetic ketamine and altered states of consciousness in humans. _Br. J. Anaesth._ 121, 249–259
(2018). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Pechadre, J. C. et al. Comparison of the central and peripheral effects of cetirizine and terfenadine. _Eur. J. Clin. Pharm._
35, 255–259 (1988). Article CAS Google Scholar * Roopun, A. K. et al. Cholinergic neuromodulation controls directed temporal communication in neocortex in vitro. _Front. Neural Circuits_
4, 8 (2010). PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Carracedo, L. M. et al. A neocortical delta rhythm facilitates reciprocal interlaminar interactions via nested theta rhythms. _J.
Neurosci._ 33, 10750–10761 (2013). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Nevian, T., Larkum, M. E., Polsky, A. & Schiller, J. Properties of basal dendrites of layer 5
pyramidal neurons: a direct patch-clamp recording study. _Nat. Neurosci._ 10, 206–214 (2007). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Larkum, M. E., Nevian, T., Sandler, M., Polsky, A. &
Schiller, J. Synaptic integration in tuft dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons: a new unifying principle. _Science_ 325, 756–760 (2009). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Takahashi,
N., Oertner, T. G., Hegemann, P. & Larkum, M. E. Active cortical dendrites modulate perception. _Science_ 354, 1587–1590 (2016). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Binshtok, A. M.,
Fleidervish, I. A., Sprengel, R. & Gutnick, M. J. NMDA receptors in layer 4 spiny stellate cells of the mouse barrel cortex contain the NR2C subunit. _J. Neurosci._ 26, 708–715 (2006).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Monyer, H., Burnashev, N., Laurie, D. J., Sakmann, B. & Seeburg, P. H. Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and
functional properties of four NMDA receptors. _Neuron_ 12, 529–540 (1994). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Major, G., Larkum, M. E. & Schiller, J. Active properties of
neocortical pyramidal neuron dendrites. _Annu Rev. Neurosci._ 36, 1–24 (2013). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Patneau, D. K. & Mayer, M. L. Structure-activity relationships for
amino acid transmitter candidates acting at _N_-methyl-d-aspartate and quisqualate receptors. _J. Neurosci._ 10, 2385–2399 (1990). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar *
Paoletti, P., Bellone, C. & Zhou, Q. NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity and disease. _Nat. Rev. Neurosci._ 14, 383–400 (2013). Article
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Spruston, N., Schiller, J., Stuart, G. & Sakmann, B. Activity-dependent action potential invasion and calcium influx into hippocampal CA1 dendrites.
_Science_ 268, 297–300 (1995). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Mao, R. et al. Influence of a subtype of inhibitory interneuron on stimulus-specific responses in visual cortex.
_Cereb. Cortex_ 22, 493–508 (2012). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Kisvárday, Z. F., Martin, K. A., Whitteridge, D. & Somogyi, P. Synaptic connections of intracellularly filled
clutch cells: a type of small basket cell in the visual cortex of the cat. _J. Comp. Neurol._ 241, 111–137 (1985). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Veit, J., Hakim, R., Jadi, M. P.,
Sejnowski, T. J. & Adesnik, H. Cortical gamma band synchronization through somatostatin interneurons. _Nat. Neurosci._ 20, 951–959 (2007). Article CAS Google Scholar * von Engelhardt,
J. et al. GluN2D-containing NMDA receptors-mediate synaptic currents in hippocampal interneurons and pyramidal cells in juvenile mice. _Front. Cell Neurosci._ 25, 95 (2015). Google Scholar
* Middleton, S. et al. NMDA receptor-dependent switching between different gamma rhythm-generating microcircuits in entorhinal cortex. _Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 105, 18572–18577 (2008).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Saganich, M. J. et al. Cloning of components of a novel subthreshold-activating K(+) channel with a unique pattern of expression in
the cerebral cortex. _J. Neurosci._ 19, 10789–10802 (1999). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Deschênes, M., Roy, J. P. & Steriade, M. Thalamic bursting mechanism:
an inward slow current revealed by membrane hyperpolarization. _Brain Res._ 239, 289–293 (1982). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Foxe, J. J. & Snyder, A. C. The role of alpha-band
brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. _Front. Psychol._ 2, 154 (2011). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Houghton, G. &
Tipper, S. P. Inhibitory mechanisms of neural and cognitive control: applications to selective attention and sequential action. _Brain Cogn._ 30, 20–43 (1996). Article CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P. & Hanslmayr, S. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. _Brain Res. Rev._ 53, 63–8860 (2007). Article PubMed Google Scholar *
Klimesch, W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. _Brain Res. Rev._ 29, 169–195 (1999). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Callaway, E. M. & Borrell, V. Developmental sculpting of dendritic morphology of layer 4 neurons in visual cortex: influence of retinal input. _J. Neurosci._ 31, 7456–7470 (2011).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Staiger, J. F. et al. Functional diversity of layer IV spiny neurons in rat somatosensory cortex: quantitative morphology of
electrophysiologically characterized and biocytin labeled cells. _Cereb. Cortex_ 14, 690–701 (2004). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Fingelkurts, A. A. & Fingelkurts, A. A. Persistent
operational synchrony within brain default-mode network and self-processing operations in healthy subjects. _Brain Cogn._ 75, 79–90 (2011). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Pettersen, K.
H., Devor, A., Ulbert, I., Dale, A. M. & Einevoll, G. T. Current source dendity estimation based on inversion of electrostatic forward solution: effects of finite extent of neuronal
activity and conductivity discontinuities. _J. Neurosci. Meth_ 154, 116–133 (2006). Article Google Scholar * Traub, R. D. et al. Single-column thalamocortical network model exhibiting
gamma oscillations, sleep spindles, and epileptogenic bursts. _J. Neurophysiol._ 93, 2194–2232 (2005). Article PubMed Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study was
funded by The Wellcome Trust, IBM and NIH/NINDS (RO1NS044133). AUTHOR INFORMATION Author notes * These authors jointly supervised this work: Roger D. Traub, Miles A. Whittington. AUTHORS AND
AFFILIATIONS * IBM T.J. Watson Research Center, Yorktown Heights, NY, 10598, USA Roger D. Traub * Department of Neurology, Columbia University, Columbia, NY, 10032, USA Roger D. Traub *
HYMS, University of York, Heslington, YO10 5DD, UK Karen Hawkins, Natalie E. Adams, Stephen P. Hall, Anna Simon & Miles A. Whittington Authors * Roger D. Traub View author publications
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Karen Hawkins View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Natalie E. Adams View
author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Stephen P. Hall View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Anna
Simon View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Miles A. Whittington View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google
Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS K.H., N.E.A., S.P.H., S.A. and M.A.W. acquired and analysed data. R.D.T. performed simulations. R.D.T. and M.A.W. conceived and designed the study. R.D.T. and M.A.W.
wrote the MS. CORRESPONDING AUTHORS Correspondence to Roger D. Traub or Miles A. Whittington. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The authors declare no competing interests. ADDITIONAL
INFORMATION PUBLISHER’S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION REPORTING
SUMMARY PEER REVIEW FILE RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation,
distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and
indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to
the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE
CITE THIS ARTICLE Traub, R.D., Hawkins, K., Adams, N.E. _et al._ Layer 4 pyramidal neuron dendritic bursting underlies a post-stimulus visual cortical alpha rhythm. _Commun Biol_ 3, 230
(2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-0947-8 Download citation * Received: 12 December 2019 * Accepted: 20 April 2020 * Published: 11 May 2020 * DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-0947-8 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not
currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative