
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
ABSTRACT Social traditions based on communication signals are widespread in birds, cetaceans and humans, but surprisingly rare in nonhuman primates known for having genetically-determined
vocal repertoires. This study presents the first description of a singular case of behaviour associated with calling (placing a hand in front of the mouth while vocalizing: HFM) in black
howler monkeys. We showed, first, that HFM was found only in a subset of the groups observed, at the same geographical location and was age- and sex-specific. There was an audience effect on
HFM, with highest rates when a neighbouring group was visible. HFM was non-randomly combined with audio-visual signals and always performed while roaring. High HFM rates triggered more
vocal responses from group members and male neighbours and HFM signalers temporally synchronized their behaviour in a predictable way. Finally, the positioning of the hand systematically
modified the call’s auditory structure. Altogether these results support the idea that HFM is an innovated, culturally transmitted communication signal that may play a role in inter-group
competition and intra-group cohesion. This study opens new lines of research about how nonhuman primates developed strategies to overcome their constraints in acoustic plasticity very early
in the primate lineage. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS SOCIALITY PREDICTS ORANGUTAN VOCAL PHENOTYPE Article Open access 21 March 2022 THE SOUND OF YAWNS MAKES GELADAS YAWN Article
Open access 07 January 2024 FIRST EVIDENCE OF YAWN CONTAGION IN A WILD MONKEY SPECIES Article Open access 09 September 2021 INTRODUCTION Culture is a powerful tool for survival and an
important aspect of human societies. Authors have claimed that carrying out comparative studies with animals may help understand culture’s evolutionary roots1. Investigations of
intraspecific behavioural variability in animals show that a behavioural trait can be present in some populations or groups but absent in others; well-known examples include milk-bottle
opening by tits2 and tool manufacturing by chimpanzees3. When behavioural differences between populations cannot be explained by habitat or genetic variations, they may represent
socially-transmitted “traditions”4. Perry and his colleagues listed three criteria for traditions: 1) _intergroup variation_: the behaviour must be present in at least one social group and
absent in at least one other group, 2) e_xpansion_: the number of performers must increase over time and 3) _durability_: the behaviour must be long-lasting in the group’s repertoire5.
Culture and language in humans are inseparable evolutionally speaking6 and in nonhuman animals social traditions also play an important role in the evolution of communication, for example
triggering dialectal changes in the vocalization patterns of birds7 and whales8. Given their phylogenetic proximity to humans, nonhuman primates are interesting models for comparative work.
However, up to now studies have focused mainly on foraging techniques9, leaving traditions in communication signals by non-human primates less explored. One reason may be that non-human
primates are known to be strongly constrained vocally. Vocal repertoires in those species are mostly genetically determined and cases of social influences on acoustic plasticity are rare10.
Nevertheless, a few cases of vocal communication traditions by apes (e.g., nest building calls in free-ranging orang-utans11) and Old World monkeys (e.g., human alarm call in captive
Campbell’s monkeys12) have been reported. More examples include visual communication traditions by apes (e.g. handclasp grooming by chimpanzees and beckoning gesture by
bonobos13,14,15,16,17) and Old World monkeys (“hand extension” by mandrills18). Some examples involve non-random innovated associations between auditory and visual signals in both apes
(raspberry call and extended grunt associated with begging gesture in chimpanzees19) and Old World monkeys (ventro-ventral “rocking-embrace” signal always accompanied by lip smacking and
sometimes by girney call in macaques20). We investigated the potential existence of tradition based on communication signals at a more basal level of the primate phylogeny, i.e., a New World
monkey species. Our study subjects were black howler monkeys (_Alouatta pigra_), well known for their long and loud call bouts, frequently accompanied by visual displays (such as aggressive
body shakes)21,22. Despite their evident communicative abilities23, howler monkeys’ communication signals have been considered relatively inflexible24. However, recently we reported that
black howler monkeys in Palenque National Park (southern Mexico) learn to recognize individual acoustic signatures25. During that playback study, we observed that some individuals sometimes
placed one hand in front of their mouths while vocalizing (named _Hand-Front-Mouth_ [HFM], Fig. 1). As this HFM behaviour has not yet been reported for any howler monkey species, we here
evaluate its value as a cultural behaviour and as a communication signal. In particular, (1) we documented the distribution of HFM behaviours in more or less geographically distant groups in
Mexico, as well as among group members of various ages and sexes; (2) we estimated the contextual non-randomness of HFM production by studying its association with previously described
communication signals (e.g., roar calls or body shakes, displays frequently used during inter-group competition21,26) and by assessing a potential audience effect on HFM rates in the
neighbouring home range; (3) we tested whether HFM triggered a particular vocal response from intra- and inter-group receivers of both sexes; (4) we assessed the level of social coordination
and synchronization between HFM signalers within a group; and (5) we tested whether (and how) HFM modified the acoustic structure of the associated call. RESULTS DISTRIBUTION OF HFM ACROSS
GROUPS AND GROUP MEMBERS Observations focused on 19 groups and two solitary male black howler monkeys in three different geographic locations in Mexico (Table 1). However, only a subset of
the groups, geographically close to each other, displayed HFM. Monkeys in six of the eight Palenque National Park groups (PNP) and in one of the seven groups living nearby in the Palenque
fragmented forest (PFF) produced HFM behaviours, but none did so in the geographically distant Yucatan peninsula captive groups (YP) (Table 2). HFM production was age- and sex-specific; only
adult and subadult males in these seven groups performed HFM behaviours. When HFM behaviours were observed in a group, all males of that group were systematically HFM signalers. Despite the
fact that our sampling effort varied greatly among groups, we found no correlation between number of howling sequences sampled and number of sequences including HFM signals (Spearman test,
N = 21, r = 0.5561, P = 0.07). Therefore, our sampling effort did not affect the presence or absence of HFM in the study groups (i.e., it is unlikely that the absence of HFM behaviour in
certain groups was due simply to insufficient sampling). CONTEXT OF HFM SIGNALING We calculated the individual rate per minute of HFM behaviours within howling sequences for three groups
(PNP1 − N = 24 sequences, PNP2 − N = 19 and PFF2 − N = 10), which we had the opportunity to observe over long periods with an acoustic recording apparatus (see the method section for
details), (Table 2). Rates did not exceed one behaviour per minute: PNP1 – M1: 0.51 ± 0.37, M2: 0.42 ± 0.40, M3: 0.11 ± 0.16; PNP2 – M1: 0.56 ± 0.50, M2: 0.50 ± 0.43, M3: 0.07 ± 0.19; PFF2 –
M1: 0.97 ± 1.32; M1, 2 and 3 being different males per group). The average number of calls emitted per sequence (178 ± 120) was about 15 times higher than the average number of HFM produced
per sequence (11.7 ± 10.7). Howling sequences are composed of different concatenated call types. Nevertheless, 100% of the HFM signals observed were produced at the same time as a roar call
(N = 646/21170), with no HFM performed during barks (N = 0/28151), grunts (N = 0/1565) or between calls (i.e. silence gaps). HFM rates increased in the presence of other groups (i.e.,
showed an audience effect) in the neighbouring home range (Z = 39.25, P = 0.037), independently in the three studied groups (Z = 36.53, P = 0.26) (Fig. 2). HFM rates were higher when another
group was close by (i.e. auditory and visual contact, 20.3 ± 12.4% of the contexts sampled per group) than when a group was farther away (only auditory contact, P = 0.04) or when no other
group was around (Post-hoc pairwise comparisons, applying Holm correction for multiple testing: P = 0.01. There was no difference in HFM rates when neighbours were far way or absent (P =
0.21). Individuals produced more HFM behaviours when they were calling from peripheral positions (N = 24; 1.45 ± 1.08) than when they were calling from central (N = 26; 0.66 ± 0.82)
positions in their home range (Z = 42.77, P = 0.005), regardless of group identity (Z = 46.76, P = 0.14). The level of group dispersion also influenced HFM rates significantly (Z = 36.2, P =
0.04, no group effect: Z = 39.2, P = 0.6); HFM rates were higher when group members were not dispersed (N = 31; 1.22 ± 1.05 HFM/min) than when they were dispersed (N = 19; 0.54 ± 0.51
HFM/min). Also, HFM occurrence was significantly associated with “body shake” displays during howling sequences (N = 50; Fisher’s exact test: P = 0.0063). RECEIVERS’ VOCAL RESPONSES We found
a sex-dependent influence of HFM during howling sequences on the vocal activity of group members and neighbours. HFM rates were positively correlated with the number of calls produced by
neighbouring males in the corresponding sequence (N = 19; Spearman test: r = 2.89; P < 0.001), but not by neighbouring females (N = 19; r = 1.6; P = 0.13). Hence, the more frequent HFM
were in a sequence, the more calls were produced by neighbouring males. Within groups, no correlation was found with male (N = 50; r = 0.22; P = 0.12) or female (N = 50; r = 0.16; P = 0.25)
calling rates. However, HFM rates were still related to their calling quantity, as sequences with high (i.e. above the median) HFM rates were associated with more male (N = 50; Binomial
test: P < 0.001) and female (P = 0.02) calls than sequences with low (below the median) HFM rates. Interestingly the influence of HFM on calling is supported by the fact that HFM were
more frequent in the first half of the vocal sequences than in the second half (N = 50; sign test P = 0.03). SOCIAL COORDINATION BETWEEN HFM SIGNALERS First, a social contagion effect was
found in the six groups, including two adult males producing HFM behaviours. The number of sequences in which two males produced HFM together was significantly higher than the number of
sequences in which only one male produced HFM (Wilcoxon matched-paired signed-rank test: N = 6; Z = 2.301, P = 0.028). Second, interacting individuals matched the number of HFM behaviours
they performed in a given sequence. The numbers of HFM behaviours produced by the two signaling males in groups PNP1 and PNP2 (the only groups studied in detail with two adult males) were
positively correlated (Spearman tests, PNP1: N = 24, r = 0.92, P < 0.001; PN2: N = 19, r = 0.91, P < 0.001) (Fig. 3). Third, our data revealed a turn-taking pattern in HFM production.
Male group members synchronized their productions by alternating HFM rather than repeating their own production (Chi-squared tests, PNP1: N = 24, X = 39.7, P < 0.001; PNP2: N = 19, Z =
9.9, P < 0.001, Fig. 4). ACOUSTIC CHANGES ASSOCIATED WITH HFM Discriminant Function Analyses (DFA) showed that HFM predictably modified the acoustic structure of roar calls. The
percentage of correct classification of the two categories (roar with and without HFM) was much higher than expected by chance (i.e. 50%), as 89% of the calls were correctly assigned to
their category. MANOVA test confirmed a significant difference between calls with and calls without HFM (F20,57 = 14.59, P < 0.001, Wilk’s λ = 0.04). The cross-validated DFA, using random
calls (N = 20 calls, 10 calls per category) to build the model, confirmed the correct classification (85%). The acoustic parameter that contributed the most to the classification (more than
3 times more than all other acoustic parameters) was the 3rd Quartile Frequency (roar with HFM = 750+/− 147 Hz; roar without HFM = 862+/− 98 Hz). DISCUSSION According to Perry and his
colleagues’ definition5, HFM behaviour by black howler monkeys qualifies as a tradition. First, HFM has never been reported in other New Wold primate species, was found only in a subset of
the groups studied and was age- and sex-specific, confirming that it is not widespread in this species. Second, our data suggest expansion and social transmission of HFM: (1) when HFM was
observed in a group, all the adult and subadult male members performed this behaviour; (2) HFM was present only in groups geographically close to one another. Neighbouring groups in the
Palenque population are genetically more dissimilar than more distant groups27,28,29. This is important because it supports a cultural (non-genetically based) transmission. Third, HFM is
durable: intermittent monitoring between 2011 and 2014 confirmed that this behaviour was present throughout this period. However, a longitudinal study with continuous observations is now
needed to determine how the signal spreads among group members and groups. Traditions usually concern subsistence activities, body care, or social behaviours and they sometimes have no
apparent utility or purpose, as found in chimpanzees30. Here, we believe HFM to be a communication signal. First, HFM behaviours are involved in non-random combinations with other
communication signals. HFM are associated only with roar calls and alternate with body-shake displays. Second, HFM are produced more frequently when group members are spatially cohesive,
supporting a social context of production. Third, HFM signalers coordinate their production of HFM by performing HFM synchronously and at similar rates when together. Fourth, individuals
took turns while producing HFM behaviours. Turn-taking is one of the characteristic interaction patterns found in communication31,32, typically playing a role in the maintenance of
socio-spatial cohesion5. Fifth, HFM has an impact on the behaviours of receivers, which is an essential criterion of communication33. It increased the vocal activity of receivers in a sex-
and social-dependent way (i.e. male and female group members as well as male neighbours). Finally, an audience effect on HFM rates was found, as HFM are most frequent during inter-group
encounters. The social function of HFM signals remains an open question, deserving more detailed observations of what occurs in both emitters and receivers when a male is _vs_ is not
performing HFM. However, we believe that the HFM signal plays a role in both inter-group competition and intra-group cohesion. The former conclusion is drawn from its association with
agonistic displays (body/branch shakes) and roar calls, which are known to play a key role during agonistic inter-group encounters21,34. It is also drawn from the fact that HFM rates were
correlated with male neighbour calling effort and audience effect at the border of the home range. The latter conclusion is drawn from the observed group spatial cohesiveness, the fact that
male and female group members join the howling event more frequently and the coordination between males from the same group associated with signaling. How HFM signals should qualify is
another still open question. Two plausible hypotheses can be formulated at this stage. One hypothesis is that HFM is only an auditory signal. Placing the hand in front of the mouth would
thus be a postural innovation associated with calling. This is supported by the fact that HFM modifies the acoustic structure of the call produced. Interestingly, the main acoustic
parameters affected tend to show that HFM makes the emitter’s voice appear deeper. It is well known that lower-pitched calls travel farther and are associated with males of larger body
size35. Again, this would support an inter-group competition function. Cases of nonhuman primates using behaviours to modify their voices are extremely rare. A study of orang-utans showed
the ability of individuals to modify acoustic parameters during kiss-squeak using leaves as a tool or putting a hand to the mouth, which the authors suggested could convey (falsified)
information to the predator about their body size36. Another hypothesis is that HFM is a multimodal (audio-visual) innovated signal and that the arm position conveys extra information not
conveyed by the call. While it seems obvious that the arm position of the emitter is visible both to group members and to close neighbours, we do not have access to data showing that
receivers actually visually pay attention to the posture. Further experiments and observations are needed to sort these two hypotheses. In conclusion, we showed here a rare case of tradition
of a new behaviour associated with calling in nonhuman primates. The questions of whether this is an uni- (auditory) or multi-modal (audio-visual) signal and how this behaviour appeared and
spread, remain open, deserving further investigations. Our results suggest that HFM behaviour is more than simply a fashion without apparent function, but rather that it has some role in
intra-group and inter-group communication. Interestingly, the fact that HFM behaviour was found in New World monkeys suggests that different strategies to compensate for non-human primates’
limited acoustic plasticity may have emerged very early in that lineage37,38. METHODS STUDY SITES AND GROUPS Observations focused on 19 groups and two isolated adult male black howler
monkeys (_Alouatta pigra_) (see Table 1 for group characteristics and sampling efforts) in three different geographical areas: (1) Palenque National Park (PNP): here we observed eight
free-ranging social groups with adjacent territories at Palenque National Park, Mexico. This 17.7 km2 park includes 6.0 km2 of primary tropical continuous rain forest where these groups
live39. These groups included 1 to 3 males, 1 to 3 females and their offspring. (2) Palenque fragmented forest (PFF): here we studied seven free-ranging social groups, each one living in
its own fragmented forest patch, 10.3 ± 8.4 km from PNP. These patches included agricultural lands, urban areas with dispersed trees and dense secondary forest. These groups included 1 or 2
males, 1 to 3 females and their offspring. (3) Yucatan peninsula (YP): here we studied four social groups (composed of 1 male, 1 to 2 females and their offspring) and two isolated
individuals in captivity (Xcaret Park), 760 km from Palenque in the Yucatan peninsula of Mexico. All adult subjects were wild-born on the Yucatan peninsula (they were caught in the same
region after the destruction of their habitat by a hurricane and housed according to the original group composition). Three social groups were housed in 3 × 3 × 6 m cages and the individual
monkeys in 3 × 2 × 3 m cages, enriched with perches for climbing (all of them had auditory contact with neighbours and all but one also had visual contact). The fourth group (YP4), visually
and auditorily isolated, lived on an island (8 m diameter). Animals were fed twice a day (6 am and 6 pm with fruit and kibble). Water was available _ad libitum_. Our research complied with
protocols of the Animal Care Committee of Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico and adhered to the legal requirements of Mexico. Protocols were approved by Direccion General de Vida
Silvestre (SEMARNAT), permit SGPA/DGVS/00692/08. OBSERVATIONS, ACOUSTIC RECORDINGS AND STATISTICAL ANALYSES HFM DISTRIBUTION ACROSS GROUPS AND GROUP MEMBERS To evaluate the distribution of
HFM across groups and group members, we observed all subjects available, i.e. 19 social groups and two isolated individuals. HFM never occurred outside the long and loud howling sequences in
any of the individuals observed. To assess its distribution, a single observer (MBJ) scored, for each of the 183 howling sequences observed (Table 1), the identity of the individuals who
performed at least one HFM using the one/zero sampling method40. This was used to calculate individual HFM contribution scores (number of sequences during which an individual displayed at
least one HFM/number of vocal sequences including this individual*100). CONTEXT OF HFM SIGNALING The subsequent analyses (i.e. context of HFM signaling, receivers’ vocal responses, acoustic
changes associated with HFM) were done with the data from only three of the groups presenting HFM behaviours (i.e. PNP1, PNP2 and PFF2), because those groups could be observed in more detail
and over longer periods with an acoustic recording apparatus. A howling sequence (defined by Kitchen41 as a long-lasting howling bout with concatenation of different call types, i.e.,
roars, barks and grunts) is composed of different calls produced by different callers. Howling sequences occurred about once or twice a day per group with a within-sequence inter-call
interval of 0.54 sec +/− 0.07 sec. HFM behaviours always occurred while a signaler was producing a call. Acoustic recordings were used to ensure the classification of call types associated
with HFM. Calls were classified as barks, roars and grunts (for definitions see42,43). The recording apparatus included a directional microphone (SONY ECM-672) and a tie microphone
(EUROPSONIC ECM 104), connected to a digital audio recorder (MARANTZ PMD670) (Sample rate 44.1 kHz, resolution 16 bits, WAV format). The first track was used to record monkey calls and the
second track was used to record caller and HFM signaler identities during each howling event. All recordings were made at comparable distances from callers (20 to 30 m). For these three
groups we scored the identity of each signaler and each caller during all the howling sequences, using the all-occurrence sampling method31. The context of signaling by these three groups
was evaluated by recording during each howling sequence: - the type of audience in the neighbouring home range: absence of a neighbouring group, a neighbouring group in the distance (only
audible), or a neighbouring group nearby (audible and visible); - the position of the focus group on its home rage when a howling sequence started: peripheral (site where previous group
encounters were observed) or central (site where no group encounters had been observed previously); - dispersion of group members: not dispersed or dispersed (when more than 50% of the group
members were distributed over an area above 25 m2)44; - other behaviours: when at least one “body-shake” display, which is a discomfort and agonistic signal26,45, occurred during a howling
sequence. Using a Binomial Generalized Linear Model, we tested the influence of howling context (neighbour absent, distant, or close), group position (central, peripheral), dispersion
(dispersed or not) and identity on HFM rates (number of HFM behaviours per howling sequence/total sequence duration in minutes). A Fisher exact test was used to estimate statistically the
contextual association between HFM signals and body shakes. RECEIVERS’ VOCAL RESPONSES For all sequences in PNP1, PNP2 and PFF2 groups, we calculated the rate (per minute) of HFM production,
the number of calls emitted per male and per female within the focal group and in the neighbouring group at the time of encounters. Then, Spearman tests were done to explore the
relationships between those measures. When no correlation was found, we also used Binomial tests to compare the number of calls emitted in sequences with high (above the median) vs low
(below the median) HFM rates. SOCIAL COORDINATION BETWEEN HFM SIGNALERS First, to assess the level of social contagion among HFM signalers, based on data for all the groups (i.e. PNP1, PNP2)
producing HFM signals and including two adult males, we compared, using a Wilcoxon test, the percentages of sequences in which one _versus_ two signalers were observed. Second, based on
data for the groups sampled using the all-occurrence regime, Spearman correlation tests compared the numbers of HFM signals per sequence produced by the two contributing males. This analysis
(and the following) was done with PNP1 and PNP2, as PFF2 was a single male group. Third, we evaluated adaptation between synchronization of signalers’ behaviour and a turn-taking pattern
throughout a given sequence by comparing, using Chi-squared tests, the number of times a given male repeated his own HFM twice in a row with the number of times two males alternated HFM.
ACOUSTIC CHANGES ASSOCIATED WITH HFM Spectrograms were generated using Raven bioacoustics software (v. 1.2, Cornell Bioacoustics Laboratory), with a Fast Fourier transformation (FFT) and a
time window of 256 points. In order to estimate if HFM influenced the acoustic structure of the call produced, we measured the same eight representative acoustic parameters as in Briseño
Jaramillo _et al._25 (see definitions in Table 3): Lowest frequency (Low Freq, Hz), Highest frequency (High Freq, Hz), 1st and 3rd quartile frequency (Q1freq and Q3freq, Hz), Aggregation of
entropy (Aggr Entropy, Hz), 90% bandwidth (BW 90%, Hz), Total duration (s) and Energy (dB). We randomly selected a sample of 50 roar calls with HFM and 50 calls without HFM, produced by five
males (N = 10 calls per male for each category) from the three groups. We never selected more than three calls per sequence to limit pseudoreplication. For each acoustic parameter measured,
normality was confirmed with Shapiro tests (p > 0.05 in all cases). We also confirmed, by visually inspecting box plots, that the variances of our parameters were homogeneous. Then, we
transformed the eight acoustic variables into a set of non-correlated components using Principal Component Analyses (PCA). To determine the number of relevant components (PC) for each call
type, we used the Kaiser-Guttman criterion (keeping only PCs with eigenvalues > 1). We ran a MANOVA analysis using these components to assess the contribution of each acoustic parameter
in the discrimination between calls. To estimate the reliability of this discrimination, a first Discriminant Function Analysis (DFA) was performed on the entire data set and then a second
more conservative cross-validated DFA was done. For this second DFA, a subset of 20% of the calls (N = 10/calls per category) was randomly used for establishing the model. The remaining
calls were then used to test the classification (as in Crockford & Boesh46 and Briseño Jaramillo _et al._25). ADDITIONAL INFORMATION HOW TO CITE THIS ARTICLE: Briseño-Jaramillo, M. _et
al._ Behavioural innovation and cultural transmission of communication signal in black howler monkeys. _Sci. Rep._ 5, 13400; doi: 10.1038/srep13400 (2015). REFERENCES * Freeman, S. &
Herron, J. C. Evolutionary analysis. 4th ed (ed. Pearson Education Upper Saddle River) Ch. 3, 380–381 (NJ Pearson Prentice Hall, 2004). Google Scholar * Sherry, D. F. & Galef, Jr. B. G.
Cultural transmission without imitation: milk bottle opening by birds. Anim. Behav. 32, 937–938 (1984). Article Google Scholar * Vigliocco, G., Perniss, P. & Vinson, D. Language as a
multimodal phenomenon: implications for language learning, processing and evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369, 20130292 10.1098/rstb.2013.0292 (2014). Article Google Scholar
* Laidre, M. E. Do captive mandrills invent new gestures? Anim. Cogn. 11, 179–187 (2008). Article Google Scholar * Perry, S. et al. Social conventions in wild white-faced capuchin
monkeys. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 241–268 (2003). Article Google Scholar * Dediu, D. et al. Cultural evolution of language. in Cultural evolution: Society, technology, language and religion.
Strüngmann Forum Reports. vol. 12 (ed. Richerson, P. & Christiansen, M. ) 303–332 (MIT Press, 2013). Article Google Scholar * Adret-Hausberger, M. Temporal dynamics of dialects in the
whistled songs of starlings. Ethology 71, 140–152 (1986). Article Google Scholar * Garland, E. C. et al. Dynamic horizontal cultural transmission of humpback whale song at the ocean basin
scale. Curr. Biol. 21, 687–691 (2011). Article CAS Google Scholar * Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M. & Sol, D. Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain. Behav. Evol.
63, 233–246 (2004). Article Google Scholar * Bouchet, H., Laporte, M., Candiotti, A. & Lemasson, A. Flexibilité vocale sous influences sociales chez les primates non-humains. Rev.
Primatol. 5 (2013). * Wich, S. A. et al. Call cultures in orang-utans? PloS One 7, e36180 (2012). Article CAS ADS Google Scholar * Ouattara, K., Zuberbühler, K., N’goran, E. K., Gombert,
J.-E. & Lemasson, A. The alarm call system of female Campbell’s monkeys. Anim. Behav. 78, 35–44 (2009). Article Google Scholar * Nakamura, M. Grooming-hand-clasp in Mahale M group
chimpanzees: implications for culture in social behaviours. Behav. Divers. Chimpanzees Bonobos (ed. Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. ) Ch. 5, 71–83 (Cambridge University. Press
2002). Article Google Scholar * Genty, E. & Zuberbühler, K. Spatial reference in a bonobo gesture. Curr. Biol. 24, 1601–1605 (2014). Article CAS Google Scholar * Van Leeuwen, E. J.,
Cronin, K. A., Haun, D. B., Mundry, R. & Bodamer, M. D. Neighbouring chimpanzee communities show different preferences in social grooming behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279,
4362–4367 (2012). Article Google Scholar * Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Gestural communication of apes. Gesture 5, 41–56 (2005). Article Google Scholar * Genty, E.,
Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C. & Byrne, R. W. Gestural communication of the gorilla (_Gorilla gorilla_): repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Anim. Cogn. 12, 527–546 (2009).
Article Google Scholar * Laidre, M. E. Meaningful gesture in monkeys? Investigating whether mandrills create social culture. PloS One 6, e14610 (2011). Article CAS ADS Google Scholar *
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. & Leavens, D. A. Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim. Behav. 73, 281–286 (2007). Article
Google Scholar * Shimooka, Y. & Nakagawa, N. Functions of an unreported ‘rocking-embrace’ gesture between female Japanese Macaques (_Macaca fuscata_) in Kinkazan Island, Japan.
Primates 55, 327–335 (2014). Article Google Scholar * Baldwin, J. D. & Baldwin, J. I. Vocalizations of Howler Monkeys (_Alouatta palliata_) in Southwestern Panama. Folia Primatol. 26,
81–108 (1976). Article CAS Google Scholar * Jones, C. A possible example of coercive mating in mantled howling monkeys (_Alouatta palliata_) related to sperm competition. Neotrop Primat
10, 95–96 (2002). Google Scholar * Jones, C. B. & Van Cantfort, T. E. A schema for multimodal communication applied to male mantler howler monkeys (_Alouatta palliata_). Lab Primate
Newsl. 46, 6–7 (2007). Google Scholar * Whitehead J. M. Long-distance vocalizations and spacing in mantled howling monkeys, _Alouatta palliata_. PhD thesis, University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill. (1985). * Briseño-Jaramillo, M., Estrada, A. & Lemasson, A. Individual voice recognition and an auditory map of neighbours in free-ranging black howler monkeys (_Alouatta
pigra_). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 13–25 (2015). Article Google Scholar * Maestripieri, D., Schino, G., Aureli, F. & Troisi, A. A modest proposal: displacement activities as an
indicator of emotions in primates. Anim. Behav. 44, 967–979 (1992). Article Google Scholar * Van Belle, S., Estrada, A. & Strier, K. B. Social Relationships Among Male _Alouatta
pigra_. Int. J. Primatol. 29, 1481–1498 (2008). Article Google Scholar * Van Belle, S., Estrada, A., Strier, K. B. & Di Fiore, A. Genetic Structure and Kinship Patterns in a Population
of Black Howler Monkeys, _Alouatta pigra_, at Palenque National Park, Mexico: Genetic Population Structure of Black Howlers. Am. J. Primatol. 74, 948–957 (2012). Article Google Scholar *
Van Belle, S., Garber, P. A., Estrada, A. & Di Fiore, A. Social and genetic factors mediating male participation in collective group defence in black howler monkeys. Anim. Behav. 98,
7–17 (2014). Article Google Scholar * Van Leeuwen, E. J., Cronin, K. A. & Haun, D. B. A group-specific arbitrary tradition in chimpanzees (_Pan troglodytes_). Anim. Cogn. 17, 1421–1425
(2014). Article Google Scholar * Lemasson, A., Gandon, E. & Hausberger, M. Attention to elders’ voice in non-human primates. Biol. Lett. 6, 325–328 (2010). Article Google Scholar *
Lemasson, A. et al. Youngsters do not pay attention to conversational rules: is this so for nonhuman primates? Sci. Rep. 1, 22–25 (2011). Article CAS Google Scholar * Smith, W. J.
Message, meaning and context in ethology. Am. Nat. 99, 405–409 (1965). Article Google Scholar * Oliveira, D. A. & Ades, C. Long-distance calls in Neotropical primates. An. Acad. Bras.
Ciênc. 76, 393–398 (2004). Article Google Scholar * Lloyd, P. Pitch (F0) and formant profiles of human vowels and vowel-like baboon grunts: the role of vocalizer body size and
voice-acoustic allometry. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 117, 994–1005 (2005). Google Scholar * Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Van Schaik, C. P. & Wich, S. A. Tool use in wild orang-utans modifies
sound production: a functionally deceptive innovation? Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 276, 3689–3694 (2009). Article Google Scholar * Lemasson, A., Ouattara, K. & Zuberbüehler, K. in The
evolutionary emergence of language: evidence and inference. (eds Botha, R. & Everaert, M. ) 181–203 (Oxford University Press, 2013). * Vilain, A., Schwartz, J.-L., Abry, C. &
Vauclair, J. Primate Communication and Human Language: Vocalisation, gestures, imitation and deixis in humans and non-humans. 1, (John Benjamins Publishing, 2011). * Díaz Gallegos, J. R.
Estructura y composición florística de la vegetación del parque nacional zona arqueológica de Palenque, Chiapas, México. PhD thesis, Universidad Juarez Autonoma de Tabasco. (1996). *
Altmann, J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 49, 227–267 (1974). Article CAS Google Scholar * Kitchen, D. M. Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud
calls: effect of numeric odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Anim. Behav. 67, 125–139 (2004). Article Google Scholar * Calegaro-Marques, C. & Bicca-Marques, J.
C. Vocalizações de _Alouatta caraya_ (Primates, Cebidae). Primatol. No Braz. 5, 129–140 (1995). Google Scholar * Kitchen, D. M. Agression and assessment among social groups of belizean
black howler monkeys. PhD thesis, University of Minnesota. (2000) * Candiotti, A., Zuberbuhler, K. & Lemasson, A. Convergence and divergence in Diana monkey vocalizations. Biol. Lett. 8,
382–385 (2012). Article Google Scholar * Remedios, R., Logothetis, N. K. & Kayser, C. Monkey drumming reveals common networks for perceiving vocal and nonvocal communication sounds.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106, 18010–18015 (2009). Article CAS ADS Google Scholar * Crockford, C. & Boesch, C. Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, _Pan troglodytes_ verus:
analysis of barks. Anim. Behav. 66, 115–125 (2003). Article Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are very grateful to the Comision Nacional de Areas Naturales Protegidas
of Mexico (CONANP) for permission to work at Palenque National Park and to the Director of Wildlife at Xcaret Park for authorization to carry out research with their captive black howler
groups. We also acknowledge the Permission for the primate research program at Palenque National Park granted by the division of wildlife of the environmental agency of Mexico (Dirección
General de Vida Silvestre (SEMARNAT-Mexico); permit code SGPA/DGVS/02684/10). Our research was supported by grants from CONACYT, Graduate Program in Biological Sciences (UNAM), CNRS (PICS
program), ANR, IUF and IDEA WILD. We are grateful to The Institute of Biology (UNAM) and Animal and Human Ethology Laboratory (University of Rennes 1) staff members for administrative,
statistical (especially V. Biquand) and logistical support. We thank Master students (Mélissa Berthet and Eloise Chailleux) and assistants (Rogier Hitzen, Maggie May, Elizabeth Unger, Cindy
Maslarova and Oskar Persson) for support in the field. We thank Dr. Sarie Van Belle of the Institute of Biology of UNAM and the University of Texas-Austin for providing general contextual
information on the black howler groups studied in Palenque National Park and with whom I discussed in the first place this unusual behaviour. We are also grateful to Ann Cloarec, Lynna Kiere
and Carol Van Strum for improving the written English. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Instituto de Biología, Coyoacán, City, 04510,
México M. Briseño-Jaramillo & A. Estrada * Université de Rennes 1, Ethologie Animale et Humaine, U.M.R. 6552 - C.N.R.S, Station Biologique, Paimpont, 35380, France A. Lemasson * Institut
Universitaire de France, 103 boulevard Saint-Michel, Paris, 75005, France A. Lemasson Authors * M. Briseño-Jaramillo View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed
Google Scholar * A. Estrada View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * A. Lemasson View author publications You can also search for this author
inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS All authors designed the study and wrote the paper. M.B.J. collected the data. M.B.J. and A.L. analyzed the data. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING
INTERESTS The authors declare no competing financial interests. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the
Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Briseño-Jaramillo, M., Estrada, A. & Lemasson, A. Behavioural innovation and
cultural transmission of communication signal in black howler monkeys. _Sci Rep_ 5, 13400 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13400 Download citation * Received: 11 February 2015 * Accepted:
24 July 2015 * Published: 25 August 2015 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13400 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get
shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative